A biophysical network model reveals the link between deficient inhibitory cognitive control and major neurotransmitter and neural connectivity hypotheses in schizophrenia

07/20/2021 ∙ by Konstantinos Spiliotis, et al. ∙ University of Naples Federico II 0

We address a biophysical network dynamical model to study how the modulation of dopamine (DA) activity and related N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) glutamate receptor activity as well as the emerging Pre-Frontal Cortex (PFC) functional connectivity network (FCN) affect inhibitory cognitive function in schizophrenia in an antisaccade task. The values of the model parameters and the topology of the PFC-FCN were estimated by minimizing the differences between simulations and the observed distributions of reaction times (RT) during the performance of the antisaccade task in 30 patients with schizophrenia and 30 healthy controls. We show that the proposed model approximates remarkably well the predicted prefrontal cortical DA hypo-activity and the related NMDA receptor hypo-function as well as the FCN dysconnection pattern that are considered as the major etio-pathological hypotheses to explain cognitive deficits in schizophrenia.

READ FULL TEXT VIEW PDF
POST COMMENT

Comments

There are no comments yet.

Authors

page 1

page 3

page 5

page 6

page 11

page 14

page 15

page 16

This week in AI

Get the week's most popular data science and artificial intelligence research sent straight to your inbox every Saturday.

1 Introduction

Despite major advancements in contemporary neuroscience research, the mechanisms that underpin the pathophysiology of major psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia remain elusive. A major challenge revolves around the development of an integrated framework, which can bridge the scales where the questions are asked and the answers are required: the microscopic level (molecular genetics, neurophysiology and neuropharmacology of the neuron) and the macroscopic level where one observes and measures changes in the behavioral and cognitive functions combined with alterations in the neurophysiology and anatomical-functional imaging of the whole brain [1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7]. Between these two scales, it extends the mesoscopic scale, where one attempts to describe the activation and communication of specific neuronal networks located in different brain areas. The coordinated activity of these networks gives rise to emerging/macroscopically-experimentally observed behavior and corresponding global brain activation patterns. The mesoscopic level of description has been the focus of many studies on the electrophysiology of rodents and primates using single neuron, and more recently multi-neuron recording techniques, combined with histology and more recently optogenetics [8, 9].

One way to shed light on the mesoscopic organization and functionality is through the use of detailed biophysical neuronal network models. In general, the biophysical modelling approach is described by the following steps (see Fig. 1): a) identification of a specific behavioral/cognitive function that is deviant in schizophrenia, b) construction of a detailed biophysical neuronal model based on the knowledge acquired from detailed experiments and/or from animal studies including the identification of the neuronal networks at specific brain areas that govern the corresponding behavioral/cognitive function, c) use of the biophysical network model to study structural and functional connectivity changes as predicted by existing neuropharmacological and sub-neuronal function hypotheses, d) test how these changes have an impact on the predicted changes in the cognitive function, and finally, e) validation of the results of the neuronal network model using experimental data to test whether the presence of the predicted changes of the network model result in the same cognitive/behavioral deviance as observed e.g. in schizophrenia.

At the microscopic level, the dopamine hypothesis is a prominent theoretical framework that relates the mechanism of the antispychotic medications to psychopathology in schizophrenia [2, 10, 11]. The hypothesis suggests that hyper-dopaminergia or hyperactivity at D2-dopamine receptors in subcortical limbic system structures leads to the appearance of positive symptoms of the disorder (delusions, hallucinations) and that the hypo-dopaminergia or hypoactivity of D1-dopamine receptors in the prefrontal cortex leads to the appearance of negative symptoms and cognitive dysfunctions [10, 11]. Another prominent hypothesis, the glutamate hypothesis is linked with the functional modulation of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors [3, 2, 12, 13, 14]. NMDA receptors which are located in cortical pyramidal neurons are activated by glutamate, the main excitatory neurotransmitter in the mammalian brain. Activation of pyramidal neurons in the deep cortical layer results in the activation of GABAergic inhibitory interneurons in the cortex forming a local functional network [12, 2, 14]. The glutamate hypothesis states that a hypoactivity at NMDA receptors results in an imbalance of this excitation-inhibition network. Substances that are NMDA antagonists, such as phencyclidine (PCP) and ketamine can produce both positive symptoms and cognitive dysfunctions in healthy individuals that resemble schizophrenia, as well as worsening of such symptoms in schizophrenic patients [15, 14]. Furthermore, postmortem studies in patients have shown significant changes in NMDA receptor protein expression in the prefrontal cortex [14, 16].

Remarkably, these two phenomenological independent hypotheses might share the same pathophysiological pathway in the prefrontal cortex. The D1 receptor signaling in cortical neurons is regulated by the action of NMDA receptors and vice versa [17, 18, 10]. Activation of D1 receptors in the prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons by the agonist SKF81297 lead to an increase of the steady-state NMDA evoked current [18] and this effect is canceled by incubation of neurons in free medium. Inversely, activation of NMDA receptors by glutamate results in the recruitment of D1 receptors in cortical neurons while they don’t have any effect on the distribution of D2 receptors [17]. The dysconnection hypothesis describes the above synaptic complex: it suggests that psychosis in terms of aberrant neuromodulation of synaptic efficacy mediates the influence of intrinsic and extrinsic (long-range) connectivity [4]. It suggests that the key pathophysiology lies in the interactions between NMDA receptor functions, the modulatory effect of neurotransmittion and the mediated changes in synaptic efficacy. The NMDAR-mediated plasticity affects the functionality at the level of neuronal circuits which in turn leads to an abnormal functional integration (dysconnection) of cognitive functions among brain regions in schizophrenia.

Here, the cognitive function that we study is the inhibition of the response in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia while performing an antisaccade task. In the particular task, subjects are instructed to look at the opposite direction of a visually presented stimulus, thus exerting inhibitory control over the natural tendency to look towards the visual stimulus [19]. There is a large body of literature that confirms a deficit in this task in patients with schizophrenia compared to healthy controls [20]. More specifically, it has been reported that patients produce more erroneous responses towards the visual target than healthy controls while the response latency for correct antisaccades for these patients is longer and more variable than that observed for healthy controls [21, 20]. A large body of animal and human studies has confirmed the critical role of the prefrontal cortical areas such as the frontal eye field, the supplementary eye field and especially the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the performance of the antisaccade task [22, 23, 24]. There is also evidence that deficits in the performance of the antisaccade task in schizophrenia may be related with hypo-activity in specific areas of the prefrontal cortex that are activated during this task such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [22, 25, 20], (see also Fig. 1).

Here, building up on previous work [26, 27, 28, 29], we attempt to bridge the microscopic and macroscopic levels by addressing a multiscale neuronal network model of two brain regions, namely the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the superior colliculus (SC) for the simulation of the antisaccade task. The model incorporates competing neuronal activity at the intermediate layer of the SC that is driven both by a planned input (the antisaccade command) and a reactive input (the erroneous prosaccade command). The output of the model are the reaction time distributions for the correct antisaccades and erroneous prosaccades, as well as the percentage of the error prosaccades. The PFC activity is approximated by leaky spiking integrate and fire pyramidal neuronal dynamics as described in [26], while the connectivity of the PFC neurons is allowed to vary from a ring-like network to a completely random topology. Unlike previous modeling attempts [27, 28, 30], we did not assume any a-priori knowledge of the biophysical parameter values and the PFC neuronal connectivity structure; these are estimated in a strict numerical way, by “wrapping" around the detailed network model a numerical optimization structure, namely the Levenberg-Marquardt algorithm [31]

to fit the experimental behavioral data. Then, we used the calibrated model to bridge microscopic and macroscopic scales, namely to study how changes in the dopamine levels acting on D1 receptors and the related hypo-activity of NMDA receptors on simulated prefrontal cortical neurons (microscopic level) give rise to antisaccade performance differences in patients with schizophrenia vs. healthy controls. In addition, the fact that the detailed connectivity of the biophysical neuronal network model of the PFC was also allowed to vary, gave us the opportunity to (qualitatively) study how changes at the microscopic level can give rise to changes at the mesoscopic level (namely how the topology of the specific network changes) that in turn give rise to macroscopic changes at the experimentally observed behavioral performance. Our hypothesis is that decreased DA levels acting on D1 receptors associated with hypofunction of NMDA receptors in the PFC would predict antisaccade performance differences as observed in schizophrenia, namely a large increase in the error rate as well as an increase in the mean and variance of the reaction times for correct antisaccades. Furthermore, in accordance to the dysconnection hypothesis, we predicted that the deviances in the microscopic level would mediate the macroscopic effects via a change at the mesoscopic level, namely a change in the functional connectivity of the underlying neuronal network.

Figure 1: Schematic of the Experiment and different levels of inquiry ranging from the microscopic level: molecular genetics, neurophysiology and neuropharmacology of the neuron, to the mesoscopic level: activation and communication of specific networks of connected neurons located in different areas of the brain, for example of the PFC network (green triangles and red elipses are for pyramidal excitatory neurons and interneurons respectively), and finally to the macroscopic level: changes in the behavior and cognitive functions combined with changes in the neurophysiology and anatomical-functional imaging of the whole brain. Behavioral experiment: antisaccade task. Starting with a fixated cue centered on a screen, a peripheral stimulus (S) appears, the subject is instructed to look at the opposite direction (antisaccade) suppressing the saccadic motion (error prosaccade) exerting an inhibitory control over the natural tendency to look towards the visual stimulus.

2 Material and Methods

The Prefrontal Cortex network model

We build up on a model proposed in [26] using spiking integrate and fire neurons whose dynamics are governed by the current balance equation:

(1)

where is the membrane capacity, is the membrane potential of the th neuron, , and are the leak, injected, after hyper polarizing and depolarizing currents respectively; correspond to the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic currents, respectively.
If , a spike is being fired and the potential returns to . The leak current is given by:

(2)

where is the corresponding conductance. The after hyperpolarizing current is given by:

(3)

where is the time of the last spiking and reflects the decay time scale of the current; is the conductance and is the reverse potential.
The after depolarizing current, is due to the influx in pyramidal neurons and is modeled according to the following equations [26]:

(4)

where is the gating activation variable related to influx, as:

(5)

are constant time scales marking the onset and offset of the influx. is the reverse potential, is the conductance. The parameters and control the sharpness of the equilibrium function , while controls the calcium concentration resulting from the spike sequence [26]. In our model, the neurons are connected through a Watts and Strogatz (WS) small-world topology [32, 33, 34]. The network is reflected on the synaptic currents defined by the activation variable which for each single neuron is given by [35, 36, 37]:

(6)

where is the step function.
If and the synaptic equation takes the form:

(7)

Hence, the synapse turns off with

as time scale.
Instead, if and the synaptic equation takes the form:

(8)

The above has the solution

(9)

The above expression implies that synaptic jumps are given by .
Usually, fast synapses have , while for slow synapses and meaning that fast synapses activate and deactivate on fast time scales, while the slow synapses activate on fast and deactivate on slow time scales [35, 36]. The form of the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic currents are given, respectively, by:

(10)

for , and

(11)

for . The summation is taken over the presynaptic neurons.
Excitatory currents result from pyramidal neurons, while inhibitory currents result from inter-neurons. Out of the neurons, are pyramidal neurons, while the rest are inter-neurons. Fig.2(a) shows the dynamics of two neurons in the PFC network. The neurons although start with nearby initial conditions they exhibit slightly different firing rates (see Fig.2(b)).

(a)(c)(b)
Figure 2: Simulation of neurons in PFC and SC. (a) Membrane potentials for two neurons in the PFC network. One of these neurons fires with higher frequency (black line). (b) The corresponding linear rising phase (see sec. 2) for the previous PFC neurons. A neuron fires with slightly higher frequency (black line) giving a bigger slope in the linear frequency-time approximation. (c) Firing rates in SC of buildup and burst neurons from a simulation run of the antisaccade task. Buildup neurons (black line and blue lines) encoding reactive and planned saccade tasks. When the activity of buildup neuron crosses a threshold (400HZ red dash line) an erroneous prosaccade (light green) is initiated followed by a correct saccade.

The Superior Colliculus (Tectum) model

The neuronal model is a classic on-center off-surround leaky competitive integrator [38, 39, 40, 41]. The internal state which represents the firing rate of the th node is governed by the following differential equation:

(12)

where is the synaptic efficacy from neuron to neuron ,

is the activity function of node

, and are the reactive and planned inputs, respectively, that the Tectum receives from other cortical areas, is a global inhibition term, and is the background noise.

The value of is set to zero for the buildup neurons and to a large value for the burst neurons as burst neurons are shown to have a discharge activity only after the activity of the buildup neurons reaches a certain threshold [42]

. The activity of the burst neurons is restored back to zero only when these neurons surpass an activity level equal to 80% of their theoretical maximum discharge rate

[38, 27]. The activity function of a neuron

representing the average membrane potential is given by a sigmoid function

(13)

where is the steepness and is the offset of the sigmoid function. The interaction matrix allows for lateral interactions between neurons in the same colliculus and between neurons located in opposite colliculi sites [43, 44]; it depends only on the spatial distance and it is positive (excitatory) i.e. for short distances, and negative(inhibitory) i.e. for long distancing neurons. Thus, the interaction matrix is described by

(14)

where , , and are free parameters and and are spatial parameters.

Two competing input signals are integrated in the Tectum: a planned and a reactive. In the model, the origins of these two input signals differ: the reactive signal is considered to originate from the Post Parietal Cortex (PPC), whereas the planned signal from the frontal executive centers of the brain (the PFC). The reactive input signal is governed by a simple differential equation reading:

(15)

are the integration strengths, is a theoretical maximum allowed activity for the reactive input, is a constant indicating the onset of the incoming signal . is the strength of the rising phase and

represents the linear rising phase of the reactive input and follows a bell like probability distribution function (normal with mean value

and standard deviation

) [27, 28]. The reactive input reflects the sensory information reaching the Tectum without extensive information processing and it is taken to follow closely the onset of a visual stimulus in the periphery with a delay, .

The planned input signal has also a linear rising phase [45] before it reaches its theoretical maximum value and is governed by:

(16)

is the theoretical maximum activity of the planned input, is the strength of the planned input and is the slope of the planned input rising phase and it is extracted from the neural integration (time evolution) of the PFC according to the following way: for each PFC neuron, we perform a linear approximation of the corresponding firing rate (which is called linear rising phase and it is calculated in the first 70 msec). The mean value of these slopes defines . The planned input reflects the processing of the planned antisaccade by higher processing centers such as the frontal eye fields (FEF), the supplementary eye fields (SEF), and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) to determine the behavioral response that would be appropriate for the given task instruction and it is considered to take longer, i.e. for processing than the reactive input due to additional cortical processing.

The strength of both planned and reactive signals is given by:

(17)

where and are the indices of nodes and is the standard deviation of the Gaussian. The width of the Gaussian function was derived from the shape of movement fields of saccade related neurons in the monkey SC [42, 38]. In the simulations, the threshold level (horizontal line at about 400 Hz) was appropriately adjusted, so that both buildup neurons encoding the reactive and planned inputs cross the threshold and an erroneous prosaccade (error burst) was initiated followed by a correct saccade (correct burst). We considered a correct antisaccade response, if the movement of the eyes went in the opposite direction of the stimulus; all other cases were considered to be error prosaccades. Saccade reaction times were estimated to be the time interval from the onset of peripheral stimulus until the time the activity of the burst neurons deviated from zero plus 20 ms (approximate time required for burst neuron signal to reach the eye muscles) [27, 28]. Fig.2(c) shows the firing rates in SC of two buildup and two burst neurons from a simulation run of the antisaccade task.

3 Experimental Data Analysis

Experimental Data

The experimental data used in this study are derived from a previous study of the group [46]. Briefly the antisaccade performance data of 30 male patients (age span 18-30 years) with DSM-IV schizophrenia formed the patient group. Patients were evaluated at the Psychosis Unit of the Psychiatry Department of the National and Kapodistrian University of Athens at Eginition Hospital and the diagnosis of schizophrenia was confirmed by a trained psychiatrist with the use of the Mini International Neuropyschiatric Interview (M.I.N.I., version 5.0.0., DSM IV) [47]. Exclusion criteria consisted of the following: neurological disorder (epilepsy, multiple sclerosis etc), mental retardation and drug abuse within the last year before evaluation. All patient participants were receiving antipsychotic medication and were in a stable phase of the disorder during testing.

The control sample was derived from the ASPIS (Athens Study for Psychosis proneness and Incidence of Schizophrenia) data base [48, 49, 50]. For the purposes of ASPIS oculomotor task data (smooth eye-pursuit, saccade, antisaccade, visual fixation) were collected from a population of 2120 conscripts of the Greek Air Force aged 18-25 years. Valid antisaccade data were obtained from 2006 individuals.

Oculomotor tasks were performed in a set up that has been described in detail in our previous studies [48, 49, 50]. The antisaccade task was preceded by a calibration procedure, with saccades at 5 and 10 degrees to the left and to the right of a central fixation point. Each antisaccade trial started with the appearance of a central fixation stimulus (white cross 0.3 x 0.3 degrees of visual angle). After a variable period of 1-2 sec, the central stimulus disappeared and a peripheral stimulus (same white cross) appeared randomly at one of the 9 prescribed distances (2-10 degrees at 1 degree intervals) either to the left or to the right of the central fixation stimulus. The subjects were instructed to make an eye movement to the opposite direction from that of the peripheral stimulus as quickly as possible. Each subject performed 90 trials.

An interactive PC program (created using the TestPoint CEC) was used for the detection and measurement of saccades from the eye movement record [48, 49, 50]. We excluded trials with artifacts (blinks, etc.) in the period extending from 100ms before the appearance of the peripheral target to the end of the first saccade as well as trials for which an eye movement occurred within the 100-ms period before the appearance of the peripheral target. In addition, in order to avoid including predictive movements or too slow responses, we excluded trials with reaction times that were not within the window of 80-600ms. Using the above criteria, the minimum number of valid trials retained for each subject was 30 and the maximum was 90.

(a)(b)(c)
Figure 3: Healthy controls. Distributions of reaction times (RT) during the experiment (see sec.  3). (a)

Distribution of RT which includes correct antisaccades and wrong prosaccades; the main statistical moments are found to be: mean value

, standard deviation . (b) Distribution of antisccade only RT: mean value and standard deviation . (c) Distribution of error prosaccases RT: mean value and standard deviation .
(a)(b)(c)
Figure 4: Distributions of reactions times (RT) of schizophrenic patients during the experiment (see sec. 3). (a) Distribution of RT which includes correct antisaccades and wrong prosaccades: mean value , standard deviation . (b) Distribution of antisccade only RT: , standard deviation . (c) Distribution of error prosaccases RT: mean value , standard deviation .

Data Analysis

The error rate was computed for all patients and healthy controls as the percentage of erroneous prosaccades towards the peripheral target over all valid trials. The mean value and standard deviation for each group was then computed and compared with the error rate of the simulation data using the t-test at a 0.05 level of significance.

The reaction times for valid trials for each subject were used to derive the cumulative reaction time distribution for each subject. Then, the reaction times for correct antisaccades and erroneous prosaccades were processed separately for each subject to derive the corresponding cumulative reaction time (RT) distributions. For all RT distributions, we also calculated the percentiles (from 5% to 95% with a step of 5%). The percentiles were then averaged across the group to give the average group percentiles that are plotted in the average cumulative distribution. Ratcliff [51] showed that this average distribution retains the basic shape characteristics of the individual distributions. Thus, we created experimental averages of the RT distributions for (a) all RT, (b) correct antisaccade RT only, and (c) error prosaccade RT. Fig. 3 depicts the resulting experimental RT distributions for the healthy controls, while Fig. 4 illustrates the RT distributions for the patients. The correct antisaccade cumulative RT distributions for each group were then compared with the corresponding average cumulative RT distributions derived from the model simulation using the Kolmogoroff-Smirnov test at a 0.05 significance level.

Numerical Optimization

Our PFC neuronal network consisted of pyramidal neurons and inter-neurons. The SC neuronal network had neurons (15 buildup neurons, 15 burst neurons and 1 fixation neuron). Ensembles of the PFC network topology were constructed using the WS algorithm [32, 34, 33]

, thus allowing the connectivity to vary between ring and random regular structures; the connectivity topology is adjusted by the switching probability

. In line with biological theories, variations in the receptors functionality (DA, NMDA) as well as alterations in the PFC network topology are reflected in the conductances of the synaptic currents as well as in the ionic currents e.g. calcium-activated potassium [52]. Here, we investigated these alterations in the conductances of currents and by extracting the conductance

from a normal distribution with mean value

and standard deviation , while the was our third parameter to be tuned.

For each set of the model parameters , the dynamic model produces a different PFC activity (defining a distribution of trials), thus resulting to different values of the . The resulting distributed values of over all trials are then fed into the SC model in order to produce the reaction times for correct antisaccades as well error prosaccades RT. The number of simulation trials was set to .

The values of the model parameters , as well as the switching probability for the construction of the network topology were estimated numerically by minimizing the residual of the objective function defined as the norm difference of the distribution functions of the RT (both antsaccade and error prosaccade) between experimental and those resulting from simulations, i.e.:

(18)

where the norm is the Euclidean distance between the points of approximate and experimental RT distributions functions. The procedure was implemented separately in the two groups of healthy controls and patients. The minimization problem was solved using the matlab implementation of the Levenberg-Marquardt algorithm (function nlqnonlin [53])[31]. The step size tolerance was set to and the function tolerance was set to tolF=0.01. The maximum number of iterations was set to 100.

Results

Healthy controls

The numerical optimization for the healthy controls resulted to the following values of the model parameters: (95% CI: 1.986, 2.283), (95% CI: 0.591, 0.672) () and (95% CI: 1.197, 1.47), (), while for the network’s topology, the switching probability was found to be , see also tab. 1. The residual of the objective function was 0.0022. For the optimal parameter values, the model predicted an error rate of 34% for the control group which was not significantly different from the experimentally observed one at a 0.05 significance level. Fig.5 depicts the distribution function of reaction times for the experimentally observed and those derived by simulations.

(a)(b)(c)
Figure 5: The computed distributions of reactions times (RT) for healthy controls (brown color), using the value parameters estimated by numerical optimization. The experimental distributions are also depicted for comparison purposes. (a) Distribution of all RT (correct antisaccades and error prosaccades): mean value , standard deviation ( and are the corresponding experimental values). (b) Distribution of RT only for the correct antisaccades: mean value , standard deviation ( and are the corresponding experimental values).(c) Distribution of error prosaccades RT: mean value , standard deviation ( and are the corresponding experimental values).
Parameter Estimated values

95% Confidence Intervals

1. 2.135
2. 0.631
3. 1.33
4. 0.0101

Table 1: Estimated values of model parameters along with their 95% confidence intervals for the healthy controls.

In order to compare between the experimental and computational RT distributions, we performed a Kolomogorov-Smirnov test. The statistical test showed that the null Hypothesis that the RT data come from the same population cannot be rejected at a 0.05 significance level (p-value: 0.77).

Patients with Schizophrenia

The resulting optimal values for the group of patients were: for the switching probability. The residual of the objective function was 0.003. With these parameter values, the model predicted an error rate of 44% for the group of patients, which was not significantly different from the experimentally observed one. Fig.6 depicts the distribution of RT antisaccades for the group of patients. Experimental and model-derived RT are in a good agreement. By applying the Kolmogorov Smirnov test, it results that the hypothesis that the two sets of data come from the same distribution cannot be rejected at the 0.05 significance level (p-value: 0.543).

(a)(b)(c)
Figure 6: Computed distributions of reactions times (RT) for patients as derived using the parameter values estimated by numerical optimization (brown color). The experimental distributions are also depicted for comparison purposes (blue color). (a) Distribution of all RT (correct antisaccades and error prosaccades). Error prosaccades: mean value , standard deviation (the corresponding experimental statistical moments are ). (b) Distribution of antisaccade only RT: mean values , standard deviation (the corresponding experimental statistical moments are: ). (c) Distribution of error prosaccades RT: mean value , standard deviation .

Figs. 7 and 8 depict the mean and 95% confidence interval estimations of the RT. Fig. 9

shows a schematic of two characteristic PFC neural networks for the healthy controls (Fig.

9(a)) and for the group of patients (Fig.9 (b)); in the second case the resulting PFC network is found to be more random with an increased number of random remote connections. It has been suggested that such random structures act as communication-integration disruptors [1, 54]. In concurrent, Fig.9(c) depicts a schematic of a local circuit between a pyramidal excitatory PFC neuron and a GABAergic interneuron (microscopic scale). The optimization process manifested here a mechanism by which the dopamine D1 results to a hypofunction of NMDA neurons (lower in patient case) disrupting this way the functionality of NMDA-GABAergic dipoles. In the higher level of the neural PFC network (mesoscopic scale) (see Fig.9(d)), the abnormal behavior of the pyramidal-interneuron circuit together with the network disruption leads to an ineffective behaviour performance at the macroscopic level.

(a)(b)(c)
Figure 7: Healthy controls. The computed distributions of reaction times (solid line); the shaded area depicts the 95% confidence intervals. (a) Distribution of all reaction times (RT) (correct antisaccades and wrong prosaccades). (b) Distribution of antisaccades only RT. (c) Distribution of error prosaccades RT.
(a)(b)(c)
Figure 8: Schizophrenic patients. Computed distributions of reaction times (solid line); the shaded area depicts the 95% confidence intervals. (a) Distribution of all reaction times (RT) (correct antisaccades and wrong prosaccades). (b) Distribution of antisccade only RT. (c) Distribution of error prosaccades RT.
Parameter Estimated value 95% Confidence Intervals
1. 2.456
2. 0.885
3. 0.957
4. 0.02

Table 2: Estimated values of model parameters along with their 95% confidence intervals for the group of patients with schizophrenia.

Discussion

Numerical simulations of the proposed PFC-SC biophysical-based network model that was developed to assess the antisaccade performance in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia predicted the hypothesis about the effects of the changes in the dopamine levels, NMDA functionality and simultaneously the alteration of the PFC network topology. In particular, decreased DA levels in the PFC, combined with the NMDA hypofunction and the connectivity topology alteration predicted all basic antisaccade performance differences as have been observed experimentally in patients with schizophrenia (see introduction), namely a large increase in the error rate as well as an increase in the mean and variance of the reaction times for the correct antisaccades. Here, it is important to stress that the numerical optimization for the calibration of the model parameters was performed based on the total experimental distribution which contains both antisaccade and error prosaccade RT. Simulation results were in a very good agreement with the experimentally observed ones (both antisaccades and error prosaccades), thus providing evidence that the proposed model is able to capture the essential aspects of the observed data.

The estimated values of the model parameters for the group of patients suggest a deviant in the PFC activity. More specifically, the conductance of the inhibitory current current was found higher in the group of patients (2.456 in patients vs. 2.135 in healthy controls) suggesting an inhibition on the PFC neurons. The increment of decreases the firing rates of the PFC neurons which in turn results to a lower value of the (see Fig. 2(b)). This suggests that the buildup neurons trigger more slowly, i.e. they delay to reach the critical value that governs the RT, thus leading to larger reaction times. This result is consistent with the dopamine hypo-activity hypothesis [55, 52, 56]. In [55], using cell patch clamp recording in rat prefrontal neurons showed that the DA activation (under the D1 receptor) suppress the current, thus leading to a higher spiking activity. Pezedrani et al.[56] also reports similar results regarding the Hippocampus neurons.

The conductance of the excitatory synaptic current for the group of patients was found smaller with respect to the one for the group of healthy control (0.957 in patients vs 1.33 in healthy controls), which is consistent with the glutamate hypofunction theory. The reduction of the NMDA conductance weakens the current transmission, impairs the neurons interactions, thus resulting to smaller firing rates of the PFC neurons. The decreased PFC activity weakens the planned input to SC (see sec.2). As a consequence, the planned antisaccade processing that would be appropriate for the given task instruction is defective and the SC produces longer and more variant reaction times.

(a)(b)(c)(d)D1 HypoactivityNMDA Hypofunction
Figure 9: A schematic of the PFC neural network. (a) A low number of remote connections is characteristic of the topology for the group of healthy controls. (b) A more random topology, i.e. an increased number of remote connections, resulting from larger values of the switching probability in the Watts and Strogatz [32, 33] model is characteristic for the group of schizophrenic patients. (c) A schematic of a disrupted circuit model in patients with schizophrenia. The circuit consists of cortical pyramidal neurons (green) and GABAergic neurons. A D1 hypofunction implies NMDA receptor hypofunction which results to a hypoactivity of the cortical pyramidal neurons. Due to this, the communication between pyramidal and interneurons is disrupted. (d) A schematic of the PFC network; an abnormal behavior of the pyramidal-interneuron circuit in conjunction with the network disruption (randomization) leads to ineffective performance of the PFC which is linked to the SC network.

Furthermore, the results of our study coincide with the disconnetivity hypothesis, that for the pathological case one would expect a disrupted neuronal-functional connectivity in the the PFC-SC network. In our model, the PFC was modeled by leaky integrate-and-fire dynamics evolving on a small-world network structure. In the case of the healthy control group, the value of the switching probability that determines the topology of the small-world network dictates a high clustered (similar to a ring) structure. Instead, in the group of patients, the switching probability was higher: vs for the group of healthy-control dictating a connectivity that is closer to a random structure. In the review article [57]

(and the references therein), the authors report that under working memory tasks, schizophrenic patients involve a sparser network of cortical regions resulting to a reduced cortical signal to noise ratio (which can be interpreted as a loss of signal information). Disruptions in connectivity were also reported in auditory hallucinations, compared with healthy controls and patients who had schizophrenia but not hallucinations

[58, 59], suggesting that the auditory hallucinations originate from altered connectivity of the same regions that process normal hearing and speech [1]. Similar results have been reported by Rubinov et al. [60] who studied two groups (healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia) with resting state EEG. The authors concluded that cognitive disturbances may be due to the randomization of the underlying functional connectivity network. In Lo et al. [54] it is also reported significant randomization of global network metrics for patients with schizophrenia. These conclusions are in line with our model outcomes as changes in the network topology going from clustered and structured to more random ones result in a worse cognitive performance in the antisaccade task.

Based one the above, our proposed model and numerical results may provide useful insights regarding the organization of the connectome and its alteration, especially on the hypothesis of the “small-worldness" of key parts of the sensorimotor system (in particular of the PFC) and the study of its topological characteristics. Even though, functional neuroimaging studies have provided evidence of the small-world organization of the brain [33], the connectome and its plasticity within and between areas of the cortex such as the PFC that possess a prominent role in the voluntary control of movements including saccades and the midbrain such as the SC is still not well understood [61]. Several neuroimaging studies that have focused toward this direction have revealed that there are certain differences in the functional connectivity of the PFC between healthy controls and subjects suffering from severe mental disorders such schizophrenia [62, 63, 64]. The outcomes of our model was in line with the main findings of these studies.

In conclusion, our PFC-SC biophysical network model for the approximation and assessment of the reaction times of the antisaccade task showed that the combination of three factors, namely the DA and NMDA hypofunction and the network alteration in the PFC explain the deficits in the antisaccade performance as observed experimentally in patients with schizophrenia. Furthermore, the connectivity changes, as the disconnection hypothesis suggests in the PFC connectome resemble (qualitatively) those observed in functional connectivity neuroimaging studies.

Another direction as to which the proposed model could be exploited in future studies is the investigation of the so-called Stochastic Resonance mechanism [65, 66, 67], which is related to improved reaction performance in the presence of a low level background noise within a certain interval. Future research could be also targeted on the study of the potential effects of DA-D2 receptors on the neuronal responses of pyramidal neurons in the cortical modules and subsequently on the behavioral data of the patient group. D2 receptors tend to have opposing effects relative to the action of the D1 component, as far as the enhancement or inhibition of cortical currents is concerned [68]. In addition, the extension of the model to include the cortico-striatal, the basal ganglia network constitutes another future direction. Studies in neurological inhibitory control, suggest that inhibition is achieved via a fronto-basal-ganglia network, which could intercept the Go process and thus decrease thalamocortical output [69, 70, 71]. The augmented model will be used to simulate the control override and inhibition of the habitual response selection mechanism. [72, 73, 69].

Acknowledgements

C.S. acknowledges partial support by INdAM, through GNCS research projects. K.S. and J.S. thank the DFG for support through the Collaborative Research Center CRC 1270 (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, Grant/ Award Number: SFB 1270/1–299150580).

References

  • [1] Higgins E.S. Do neural disconnects cause schizophrenia? Current Psychiatry, 6:90–96, 2007.
  • [2] T.D. Cannon. How schizophrenia develops: Cognitive and brain mechanisms underlying onset of psychosis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(12):744–756, 2015.
  • [3] S.H. Fatemi and T.D. Folsom. The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia, revisited. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(3):528–548, 2009.
  • [4] K. Friston, H.R Brown, J. Siemerkus, and K.E. Stephan. The dysconnection hypothesis. Schiz. Res., 176:83–94, 2016.
  • [5] M. Kubicki, R. McCarley, C.-F. Westin, H.-J. Park, S. Maier, R. Kikinis, F.A. Jolesz, and M.E. Shenton.

    A review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in schizophrenia.

    Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41(1-2):15–30, 2007.
  • [6] S. Ruiz, N. Birbaumer, and R. Sitaram. Abnormal neural connectivity in schizophrenia and fmri-brain-computer interface as a potential therapeutic approach. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4(MAR), 2013.
  • [7] S. Kelly, N. Jahanshad, A. Zalesky, P. Kochunov, et al. Widespread white matter microstructural differences in schizophrenia across 4322 individuals: Results from the enigma schizophrenia dti working group. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(5):1261–1269, 2018.
  • [8] J.H. Jennings, R.L. Ung, S.L. Resendez, A.M. Stamatakis, J.G. Taylor, J. Huang, K. Veleta, P.A. Kantak, M. Aita, K. Shilling-Scrivo, C. Ramakrishnan, K. Deisseroth, S. Otte, and G.D. Stuber. Visualizing hypothalamic network dynamics for appetitive and consummatory behaviors. Cell, 160(3):516–527, 2015.
  • [9] A.V. Kravitz, L.D. Tye, and A.C. Kreitzer. Distinct roles for direct and indirect pathway striatal neurons in reinforcement. Nature Neuroscience, 15(6):816–818, 2012.
  • [10] O.D. Howes and S. Kapur. The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: Version iii - the final common pathway. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(3):549–562, 2009.
  • [11] R. Brisch, A. Saniotis, R. Wolf, H. Bielau, H.G. Bernstein, J. Steiner, B. Bogerts, K. Braun, Z. Jankowski, M. Henneberg, and T. Gos. The role of dopamine in schizophrenia from a neurobiological and evolutionary perspective: Old fashioned, but still in vogue. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5(APR), 2014.
  • [12] D.A. Lewis and R.A. Sweet. Schizophrenia from a neural circuitry perspective: Advancing toward rational pharmacological therapies. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 119(4):706–716, 2009.
  • [13] G. Gonzalez-Burgos and D.A. Lewis. Nmda receptor hypofunction, parvalbumin-positive neurons, and cortical gamma oscillations in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(5):950–957, 2012.
  • [14] Wen-Jun Gao and Melissa Snyder. Nmda hypofunction as a convergence point for progression and symptoms of schizophrenia. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 7:31, 2013.
  • [15] A.C. Lahti, M.A. Weiler, T. Michaelidis, A. Parwani, and C.A. Tamminga. Effects of ketamine in normal and schizophrenic volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(4):455–467, 2001.
  • [16] Lars V Kristiansen, Ibone Huerta, Monica Beneyto, and James H Meador-Woodruff. Nmda receptors and schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 7(1):48 – 55, 2007.
  • [17] L. Scott, M.S. Kruse, H. Forssberg, H. Brismar, P. Greengard, and A. Aperia. Selective up-regulation of dopamine d1 receptors in dendritic spines by nmda receptor activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(3):1661–1664, 2002.
  • [18] G. Chen, P. Greengard, and Z. Yan. Potentiation of nmda receptor currents by dopamine d1 receptors in prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(8):2596–2600, 2004.
  • [19] P.E. Hallett. Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Research, 18(10):1279–1296, 1978.
  • [20] G. Panagiotaropoulou, E. Thrapsanioti, E. Pappa, C. Grigoras, D. Mylonas, E. Karavasilis, G. Velonakis, N. Kelekis, and N. Smyrnis. Hypo-activity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex relates to increased reaction time variability in patients with schizophrenia. NeuroImage: Clinical, 23, 2019.
  • [21] D.C. Gooding and M.A. Basso. The tell-tale tasks: A review of saccadic research in psychiatric patient populations. Brain and Cognition, 68(3):371–390, 2008.
  • [22] D.P. Munoz and S. Everling. Look away: The anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(3):218–228, 2004.
  • [23] Ch. Pierrot-Deseilligny, R.M. Müri, T. Nyffeler, and D. Milea. The role of the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1039:239–251, 2005.
  • [24] M.R.G. Brown, T. Vilis, and S. Everling. Frontoparietal activation with preparation for antisaccades. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98(3):1751–1762, 2007.
  • [25] B.C. Coe and D.P. Munoz. Mechanisms of saccade suppression revealed in the anti-saccade task. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1718), 2017.
  • [26] D. Durstewitz. Self-organizing neural integrator predicts interval times through climbing activity. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(12):5342–5353, 2003.
  • [27] V. Cutsuridis, N. Smyrnis, I. Evdokimidis, and S. Perantonis. A neural model of decision-making by the superior colicullus in an antisaccade task. Neural Networks, 20(6):690–704, 2007.
  • [28] V. Cutsuridis, I. Kahramanoglou, N. Smyrnis, I. Evdokimidis, and S. Perantonis. A biophysical neural accumulator model of decision making in an antisaccade task. Neurocomputing, 70(7-9):1390–1402, 2007.
  • [29] I. Kahramanoglou, S. Perantonis, N. Smyrnis, I. Evdokimidis, and V. Cutsuridis. Modeling the effects of dopamine on the antisaccade reaction times (asrt) of schizophrenia patients.

    Lecture Notes in Computer Science (including subseries Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence and Lecture Notes in Bioinformatics)

    , 5164 LNCS(PART 2):290–299, 2008.
  • [30] V. Cutsuridis, V. Kumari, and U. Ettinger. Antisaccade performance in schizophrenia: A neural model of decision making in the superior colliculus. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8(8 FEB), 2014.
  • [31] H.P. Gavin. The levenberg-marquardt algorithm for nonlinear least squares curve-fitting problems, 2011.
  • [32] D.J. Watts and S.H. Strogatz. Collective dynamics of ’small-world’ networks. Nature, 393(6684):440–442, 1998.
  • [33] E. Bullmore and O. Sporns. Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(3):186–198, 2009.
  • [34] C.J. Stam and J.C. Reijneveld. Graph theoretical analysis of complex networks in the brain. Nonlinear Biomedical Physics, 1, 2007.
  • [35] C.R. Laing and C.C. Chow. A spiking neuron model for binocular rivalry. Journal of Computational Neuroscience, 12(1):39–53, 2002.
  • [36] B. Ermentrout and D. Terman. Neural networks as spatio-temporal pattern-forming systems. Springer, New York, 2012.
  • [37] A. Compte, N. Brunel, P.S. Goldman-Rakic, and X.-J. Wang. Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. Cerebral Cortex, 10(9):910–923, 2000.
  • [38] T.P. Trappenberg, M.C. Dorris, D.P. Munoz, and R.M. Klein. A model of saccade initiation based on the competitive integration of exogenous and endogenous signals in the superior colliculus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(2):256–271, 2001.
  • [39] J.G. Taylor. Neural ’bubble’ dynamics in two dimensions: Foundations. Biological Cybernetics, 80(6):393–409, 1999.
  • [40] K. Arai, E.L. Keller, and J.A. Edelman. Two-dimensional neural network model of the primate saccadic system. Neural Networks, 7(6-7):1115–1135, 1994.
  • [41] Klaus Kopecz. Saccadic reaction times in gap/overlap paradigms: a model based on integration of intentional and visual information on neural, dynamic fields. Vision Research, 35(20):2911–2925, 1995.
  • [42] D.P. Munoz and R.H. Wurtz. Saccade-related activity in monkey superior colliculus i. characteristics of burst and buildup cells. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73(6):2313–2333, 1995.
  • [43] D.P. Munoz and P.J. Istvan. Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(3):1193–1209, 1998.
  • [44] M.A. Meredith and A.S. Ramoa. Intrinsic circuitry of the superior colliculus: Pharmacophysiological identification of horizontally oriented inhibitory interneurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79(3):1597–1602, 1998.
  • [45] B.A.J. Reddi and R.H.S. Carpenter. The influence of urgency on decision time. Nature Neuroscience, 3(8):827–830, 2000.
  • [46] N. Smyrnis, T. Karantinos, I. Malogiannis, C. Theleritis, A. Mantas, N.C. Stefanis, J. Hatzimanolis, and I. Evdokimidis. Larger variability of saccadic reaction times in schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Research, 168(2):129–136, 2009.
  • [47] DV Sheehan, Y Lecrubier, K Harnett Sheehan, J Janavs, E Weiller, A Keskiner, J Schinka, E Knapp, MF Sheehan, and GC Dunbar. The validity of the mini international neuropsychiatric interview (mini) according to the scid-p and its reliability. European Psychiatry, 12(5):232–241, 1997.
  • [48] N. Smyrnis, I. Evdokimidis, N. Stefanis, T. Constantinidis, D. Avramopoulos, C. Theleritis, C. Paximadis, C. Efstratiadis, G. Kastrinakis, and C. Stefanis. The antisaccade task in a sample of 2,006 young males: Ii. effects of task parameters. Experimental Brain Research, 147(1):53–63, 2002.
  • [49] N. Smyrnis, I. Evdokimidis, N.C. Stefanis, D. Avramopoulos, T.S. Constantinidis, A. Stavropoulos, and C.N. Stefanis. Antisaccade performance of 1,273 men: Effects of schizotypy, anxiety, and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(3):403–414, 2003.
  • [50] I. Evdokimidis, N. Smyrnis, T. Constantinidis, N. Stefanis, D. Avramopoulos, C. Paximadis, C. Theleritis, C. Efstratiadis, G. Kastrinakis, and C. Stefanis. The antisaccade task in a sample of 2,006 young men: I. normal population characteristics. Experimental Brain Research, 147(1):45–52, 2002.
  • [51] R. Ratcliff. Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3):446–461, 1979.
  • [52] R.C. Malenka and R.A. Nicoll. Dopamine decreases the calcium-activated afterhyperpolarization in hippocampal ca1 pyramidal cells. Brain Research, 379(2):210–215, 1986.
  • [53] matlab. fminsearch. "https://de.mathworks.com/help/matlab/ref/fminsearch.html", -.
  • [54] Chun-Yi Zac Lo, Tsung-Wei Su, Chu-Chung Huang, Chia-Chun Hung, Wei-Ling Chen, Tsuo-Hung Lan, Ching-Po Lin, and Edward T. Bullmore. Randomization and resilience of brain functional networks as systems-level endophenotypes of schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(29):9123–9128, 2015.
  • [55] F. Yi, X.-H. Zhang, C.R. Yang, and B.-M. Li. Contribution of dopamine d1/5 receptor modulation of post-spike/burst afterhyperpolarization to enhance neuronal excitability of layer v pyramidal neurons in prepubertal rat prefrontal cortex. PLoS ONE, 8(8), 2013.
  • [56] P. Pedarzani and J.F. Storm. Dopamine modulates the slow ca2+-activated k+ current i(ahp) via cyclic amp-dependent protein kinase in hippocampal neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74(6):2749–2753, 1995.
  • [57] H.-Y. Tan, J.H. Callicott, and D.R. Weinberger. Dysfunctional and compensatory prefrontal cortical systems, genes and the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Cerebral Cortex, 17(SUPPL. 1):i171–i181, 2007.
  • [58] D. Hubl, T. Koenig, W. Strik, A. Federspiel, R. Kreis, C. Boesch, S.E. Maier, G. Schroth, K. Lovblad, and T. Dierks. Pathways that make voices: White matter changes in auditory hallucinations. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(7):658–668, 2004.
  • [59] Ben Alderson-Day, Kelly Diederen, Charles Fernyhough, Judith M. Ford, Guillermo Horga, Daniel S. Margulies, Simon McCarthy-Jones, Georg Northoff, James M. Shine, Jessica Turner, Vincent van de Ven, Remko van Lutterveld, Flavie Waters, and Renaud Jardri. Auditory Hallucinations and the Brain’s Resting-State Networks: Findings and Methodological Observations. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(5):1110–1123, 06 2016.
  • [60] M. Rubinov, S.A. Knock, C.J. Stam, S. Micheloyannis, A.W.F. Harris, L.M. Williams, and M. Breakspear. Small-world properties of nonlinear brain activity in schizophrenia. Human Brain Mapping, 30(2):403–416, 2009.
  • [61] M.P.A. van Meer, W.M. Otte, K. van der Marel, C.H. Nijboer, A. Kavelaars, J.W.B. van der Sprenkel, M.A. Viergever, and R.M. Dijkhuizen. Extent of bilateral neuronal network reorganization and functional recovery in relation to stroke severity. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(13):4495–4507, 2012.
  • [62] Y. Yang, A. Raine, A.A. Joshi, S. Joshi, Y.-T. Chang, R.A. Schug, D. Wheland, R. Leahy, and K.L. Narr. Frontal information flow and connectivity in psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 201(5):408–409, 2012.
  • [63] Y. Yu, H. Shen, L.-L. Zeng, Q. Ma, and D. Hu. Convergent and divergent functional connectivity patterns in schizophrenia and depression. PLoS ONE, 8(7), 2013.
  • [64] W.-J. Gao, S.-S. Yang, N.R. Mack, and L.A. Chamberlin. Aberrant maturation and connectivity of prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia—contribution of nmda receptor development and hypofunction. Molecular Psychiatry, 2021.
  • [65] K. Wiesenfeld and F. Moss. Stochastic resonance and the benefits of noise: From ice ages to crayfish and squids. Nature, 373(6509):33–36, 1995.
  • [66] K. Kitajo, K. Yamanaka, L.M. Ward, and Y. Yamamoto. Stochastic resonance in attention control. Europhysics Letters, 76(6):1029–1035, 2006.
  • [67] I. Mendez-Balbuena, E. Manjarrez, J. Schulte-Mönting, F. Huethe, J.A. Tapia, M.-C. Hepp-Reymond, and R. Kristeva. Improved sensorimotor performance via stochastic resonance. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(36):12612–12618, 2012.
  • [68] J.K. Seamans and C.R. Yang. The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Progress in Neurobiology, 74(1):1–58, 2004.
  • [69] Adam R. Aron, Sarah Durston, Dawn M. Eagle, Gordon D. Logan, Cathy M. Stinear, and Veit Stuphorn. Converging evidence for a fronto-basal-ganglia network for inhibitory control of action and cognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(44):11860–11864, 2007.
  • [70] J.R. Wessel, N. Jenkinson, J.-S. Brittain, S.H.E.M. Voets, T.Z. Aziz, and A.R. Aron. Surprise disrupts cognition via a fronto-basal ganglia suppressive mechanism. Nature Communications, 7, 2016.
  • [71] Jonathan Heston, Alexander Friedman, Mustafa Baqai, Nicholas Bavafa, Adam R. Aron, and Thomas S. Hnasko. Activation of subthalamic nucleus stop circuit disrupts cognitive performance. eNeuro, 7(5), 2020.
  • [72] T.V. Wiecki and M.J. Frank. A computational model of inhibitory control in frontal cortex and basal ganglia. Psychological Review, 120(2):329–355, 2013.
  • [73] Brian C. Coe, Thomas Trappenberg, and Douglas P. Munoz. Modeling saccadic action selection: Cortical and basal ganglia signals coalesce in the superior colliculus. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 13:3, 2019.